Sprache · Stimme · Gehör 2019; 43(02): 93-99
DOI: 10.1055/a-0851-9063
Schwerpunktthema
© Georg Thieme Verlag KG Stuttgart · New York

Multimodale Kommunikation in frühen Buchlesesituationen und ihr Zusammenhang mit dem späteren Wortschatz

Multimodal Communication in Joint Book Reading and its Relation to Later Vocabulary Size
Angela Grimminger
Institut für Germanistik und Vergleichende Literaturwissenschaft, Psycholinguistik, Universität Paderborn
,
Katharina J. Rohlfing
Institut für Germanistik und Vergleichende Literaturwissenschaft, Psycholinguistik, Universität Paderborn
› Author Affiliations
Further Information

Publication History

Publication Date:
06 June 2019 (online)

Frühe Eltern-Kind-Interaktionen zeichnen sich durch wiederkehrende Kommunikationssituationen wie gemeinsames Buchlesen aus, in denen die Interaktionspartner lautsprachlich, aber auch nonverbal (z. B. durch Gesten) kommunizieren. Das Besondere an der Verwendung nonverbaler Mittel ist, dass sie Kindern schon früh einen Einstieg in ein kommunikatives Miteinander ermöglichen. Wir beobachteten die multimodale Kommunikation von 17 Müttern und ihren 14 Monate alten Kindern in naturalistischen Vorlesesituationen und untersuchten den Zusammenhang dieser Verhaltensweisen mit späteren Wortschatzmaßen der Kinder. Die Anzahl der Kombinationen aus Zeigegesten und Lautsprachäußerungen sowohl der Kinder als auch ihrer Mütter erklären gemeinsam 55 % der Varianz des produktiven Wortschatzes der Kinder mit 18 Monaten.

Abstract

Early parent-child interactions are characterized by repeated communicative situations such as joint book reading. In these interactions, both interaction partners communicate using verbal, but also nonverbal means such as gestures. Nonverbal means enable children from early on to take an active part in communication. In our study, we observed 17 mothers and their 14-month-old children in naturalistic joint book reading situations and analyzed their multimodal communication and its relation to the children’s later language skills. The number of gesture-and-speech combinations of both, children and their mothers, explained 55 % of the variance of the children’s productive vocabulary at 18 months.

 
  • Literatur

  • 1 Murphy CM. Pointing in the context of a shared activity. Child Dev 1978; 49: 371-380
  • 2 Rohlfing KJ, Grimminger A, Nachtigäller K. Gesturing in joint book reading. In: Kümmerling-Meibauer B, Meibauer J, Nachtigäller K. et al., eds. Learning From Picturebooks. Perspectives from Child Development & Literacy Studies. London: Routledge; 2015: 99-116
  • 3 Rossmanith N, Costall A, Reichelt AF. et al. Jointly structuring triadic spaces of meaning and action: book sharing from 3 months on. Cogn Sci 2014; 5: 1390
  • 4 Mol SE, Bus AG, de Jong MT. et al. Added Value of Dialogic Parent–Child Book Readings: A Meta-Analysis. Early Educ Dev 2008; 19: 7-26
  • 5 De Temple J, Snow CE. Learning Words from Books. In: van Kleeck A, Stahl SA, Bauer EB. eds. On Reading Books to Children: Parents and Teachers. Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum; 2003: 16-36
  • 6 Rohlfing KJ, Grimminger A, Lüke C. An Interactive View on the Development of Deictic Pointing in Infancy. Front Psychol 2017; 8: 1319
  • 7 Lüke C. Verspätete gestische Kommunikation als Vorbote von Sprachentwicklungsverzögerungen. Sprache Stimme Gehör 2019; 43: 77-85
  • 8 Colonnesi C, Stams GJJM, Koster I. et al. The relation between pointing and language development: A meta-analysis. Dev Rev 2010; 30: 352-366
  • 9 Brooks R, Meltzoff AN. Infant gaze following and pointing predict accelerated vocabulary growth through two years of age: A Longitudinal, Growth Curve Modeling Study. J Child Lang 2008; 35: 207-220
  • 10 Lüke C, Grimminger A, Rohlfing KJ. et al. In Infants’ Hands: Identification of Preverbal Infants at Risk for Primary Language Delay. Child Dev 2017; 88: 484-492
  • 11 Fasolo M, D’Odorico L. Gesture-plus-word combinations, transitional forms, and language development. Gesture 2012; 12: 1-15
  • 12 Iverson JM, Goldin-Meadow S. Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychol Sci 2005; 16: 367-371
  • 13 Rowe ML, Goldin-Meadow S. Early gesture selectively predicts later language learning. Dev Sci 2009; 12: 182-187
  • 14 Zampini L, D’Odorico L. Gesture production and language development: A longitudinal study of children with Down syndrome. Gesture 2011; 11: 174-193
  • 15 Suttora C, Salerni N. Gestural development and its relation to language acquisition in very preterm children. Infant Behav Dev 2012; 35: 429-438
  • 16 Rowe ML, Özçalışkan Ş, Goldin-Meadow S. Learning words by hand: Gesture’s role in predicting vocabulary development. First Lang 2008; 28: 182-199
  • 17 Igualada A, Bosch L, Prieto P. Language development at 18 months is related to multimodal communicative strategies at 12 months. Infant Behav Dev 2015; 39: 42-52
  • 18 Tomasello M, Carpenter M, Liszkowski U. A new look at infant pointing. Child Dev 2007; 78: 705-722
  • 19 Bruner JS. Child’s talk: Learning to use language. 1. Aufl. New York: W. W. Norton & Company; 1983
  • 20 Marcos H. How adults contribute to the development of early referential communication?. Eur J Psychol Educ 1991; 6: 271-282
  • 21 Rohlfing KJ, Wrede B, Vollmer AL, Oudeyer PY. An alternative to mapping a word onto a concept in language acquisition: Pragmatic Frames. Front Psychol 2016; 7: 470
  • 22 Marcos H, Ryckebusch C, Rabain-Jamin J. Adult responses to young children’s communicative gestures: Joint achievement of speech acts. First Lang 2003; 23: 213-237
  • 23 Goldin-Meadow S, Goodrich W, Sauer E, Iverson JM. Young children use their hands to tell their mothers what to say. Dev Sci 2007; 10: 778-785
  • 24 Laakso M, Helasvuo M-L, Savinainen-Makkonen T. Children’s early actions in learning language: A study of proto-words and pointing gestures in interaction between one-year-old child and parent. SKY J Linguist 2010; 23: 199-226
  • 25 Ritterfeld U. Welchen und wie viel Input braucht das Kind. In: Grimm H. Hrsg. Sprachentwicklung. Göttingen: Hogrefe; 2000: 403-432
  • 26 Iverson JM, Capirci O, Longobardi E, Caselli MC. Gesturing in mother-child interactions. Cogn Dev 1999; 14: 57-75
  • 27 Özçalışkan Ş, Goldin-Meadow S. Do parents lead their children by the hand?. J Child Lang 2005; 32: 481-505
  • 28 Grimminger A, Rohlfing KJ, Stenneken P. Children’s lexical skills and task demands affect gestural behavior in mothers of late-talking children and children with typical language development. Gesture 2010; 10: 251-278
  • 29 Iverson JM, Longobardi E, Spampinato K, Caselli MC. Gesture and speech in maternal input to children with Down’s syndrome. Int J Lang Commun Disord 2006; 41: 235-251
  • 30 Liszkowski U, Tomasello M. Individual differences in social, cognitive, and morphological aspects of infant pointing. Cogn Dev 2011; 26: 16-29
  • 31 Heller V, Rohlfing KJ. Reference as an Interactive Achievement: Sequential and Longitudinal Analyses of Labeling Interactions in Shared Book Reading and Free Play. Front Psychol 2017; 8: 139
  • 32 Ninio A, Bruner J. The achievement and antecedents of labelling. J Child Lang 1978; 5: 1-15
  • 33 Snow CE, Goldfield BA. Turn the page please: Situation-specific language acquisition. J Child Lang 1983; 10: 551-569
  • 34 Yont KM, Snow CE, Vernon-Feagans L. The role of context in mother-child interactions: An analysis of communicative intents expressed during toy play and book reading with 12-month-olds. J Pragmat 2003; 35: 435-454
  • 35 Grimminger A, Rohlfing KJ. Entstehung multimodaler Sprachlehrstrategien in spezifischen Interaktionen. In: Gressnich E, Müller C, Stark L. Hrsg. Lernen durch Vorlesen. Tübingen: Narr Francke Attempo; 2015: 94-109
  • 36 Nachtigäller K, Rohlfing KJ. Mothers’ talking about early object and action concepts during picture book reading. In: Kümmerling-Meibauer B. ed. Emergent Literacy: Children’s Books from 0 to 3. Amsterdam: John Benjamins; 2011: 193-208
  • 37 Grimm H, Doil H. Elternfragebögen für die Früherkennung von Risikokindern. Göttingen: Hogrefe; 2000