CC BY-NC-ND 4.0 · Sleep Sci 2021; 14(S 02): 111-117
DOI: 10.5935/1984-0063.20200088
ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Sleep fragmentation and working memory in healthy adults

Masato Okuda
1   Chubu University Graduate School of Life and Health Sciences, Department of Biomedical Sciences - Kasugai - Aichi - Japan.
,
Akiko Noda
1   Chubu University Graduate School of Life and Health Sciences, Department of Biomedical Sciences - Kasugai - Aichi - Japan.
,
Sho Mabuchi
2   Chubu University Collage of Life and Health Sciences, Department of Biomedical Sciences -Kasugai - Aichi- Japan.
,
Kunihiro Iwamoto
3   Nagoya University Graduate School of Medicine, Department of Psychiatry - Nagoya - Aichi -Japan.
,
Masahiro Banno
3   Nagoya University Graduate School of Medicine, Department of Psychiatry - Nagoya - Aichi -Japan.
4   Seichiryo Hospital, Department of Psychiatry - Nagoya - Aichi - Japan.
,
Seiko Miyata
3   Nagoya University Graduate School of Medicine, Department of Psychiatry - Nagoya - Aichi -Japan.
,
Fumihiko Yasuma
5   National Hospital Organization Suzuka Hospital, Department of Internal Medicine - Suzuka - Mie -Japan.
,
Norio Ozaki
3   Nagoya University Graduate School of Medicine, Department of Psychiatry - Nagoya - Aichi -Japan.
› Author Affiliations

Introduction Sleep is essential for performing cognitive function in humans. We have hypothesized that sleep fragmentation compared to sleep efficiency may have a negative impact on the working memory.

Material and Methods Twenty-eight healthy adults (18 males and 10 females; mean age 27.8±15.5 years) were enrolled in this study. We measured the total sleep time (TST), sleep efficiency, %stage wakefulness (W), %stage rapid eye movement (REM), %stage N1, %stage N2, %stage N3, wake after sleep onset (WASO), and arousal index using polysomnography. Working memory, executive function, and sustained attention of three domains of cognitive function were evaluated with the number of back task (N-back task), Wisconsin card sorting test (WCST), and continuous performance test-identical pairs (CPT-IP), respectively.

Results The percentage of correct answers on the 2-back task was significantly correlated with %stage REM, %stage N1, and %stage N2 (%stage REM: r=0.505, p=0.006; %stage N1: r=-0.637, p<0.001; %stage N2: r=0.670, p<0.001), and multiple regression analysis including the stepwise forward selection method revealed that %stage N2 was the most significant factor (%stage N2: β=0.670, p<0.001). The percentage of correct answers on the 2-back task was also significantly correlated with TST, sleep efficiency, WASO, and arousal index (TST: r=0.492, p=0.008; sleep efficiency: r=0.622, p<0.001; WASO: r=-0.721, p<0.001; arousal index: r=-0.656, p<0.001), and WASO was the significant factor (β=-2.086, p=0.007). The WCST category achievement and CPT-IP d-prime score were correlated with none of the sleep variables.

Conclusion Increased WASO and a decrease in %stage N2 were associated with worse working memory.



Publication History

Received: 03 September 2020

Accepted: 08 December 2020

Article published online:
30 November 2023

© 2023. Brazilian Sleep Association. This is an open access article published by Thieme under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonDerivative-NonCommercial License, permitting copying and reproduction so long as the original work is given appropriate credit. Contents may not be used for commercial purposes, or adapted, remixed, transformed or built upon. (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/)

Thieme Revinter Publicações Ltda.
Rua do Matoso 170, Rio de Janeiro, RJ, CEP 20270-135, Brazil

 
  • REFERENCES

  • 1 Diekelmann S, Born J. The memory function of sleep. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010 Feb;11(2):114-26.
  • 2 Banks S, Dinges DF. Behavioral and physiological consequences of sleep restriction. J Clin Sleep Med. 2007 Aug;3(5):519-28.
  • 3 Kronholm E, Sallinen M, Suutama T, Sulkava R, Era P, Partonen T. Self-reported sleep duration and cognitive functioning in the general population. J Sleep Res. 2009 Dec;18(4):436-46.
  • 4 Fernandez-Mendoza J, He F, Calhoun SL, Vgontzas AN, Liao D, Bixler EO. Objective short sleep duration increases the risk of all-cause mortality associated with possible vascular cognitive impairment. Sleep Health. 2020 Feb;6(1):71-8.
  • 5 Zohar D, Tzischinsky O, Epstein R, Lavie P. The effects of sleep loss on medical residents’ emotional reactions to work events: a cognitiveenergy model. Sleep. 2005 Jan;28(1):47-54.
  • 6 Rauchs G, Desgranges B, Foret J, Eustache F. The relationships between memory systems and sleep stages. J Sleep Res. 2005 Jun;14(2):123-40.
  • 7 Goerke M, Müller NG, Cohrs S. Sleep-dependent memory consolidation and its implications for psychiatry. J Neural Transm (Vienna). 2017 Feb;124(Suppl 1):163-78.
  • 8 Stickgold R. Sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Nature. 2005 Oct;437(7063):1272-8.
  • 9 Stickgold R, Whidbee D, Schirmer B, Patel V, Hobson JA. Visual discrimination task improvement: a multi-step process occurring during sleep. J Cogn Neurosci. 2000 Mar;12(2):246-54.
  • 10 Walker MP, Brakefield T, Morgan A, Hobson JA, Stickgold R. Practice with sleep makes perfect: sleep dependent motor skill learning. Neuron. 2002 Jul;35(1):205-11.
  • 11 Kato K, Iwamoto K, Kawano N, Noda Y, Ozaki N, Noda A. Differential effects of physical activity and sleep duration on cognitive function in young adults. J Sport Health Sci. 2018 Apr;7(2):227-36.
  • 12 Miyata S, Noda A, Ozaki N, Hara Y, Minoshima M, Iwamoto K, et al. Insufficient sleep impairs driving performance and cognitive function. Neurosci Lett. 2010 Jan;469(2):229-33.
  • 13 Miyata S, Noda A, Iwamoto K, Kawano N, Banno M, Tsuruta Y, et al. Impaired cortical oxygenation is related to mood disturbance resulting from three nights of sleep restriction. Sleep Biol Rhythms. 2015 Oct;13(4):387-94.
  • 14 Callicott JH, Bertolino A, Mattay VS, Langheim FJ, Duyn J, Coppola R, et al. Physiological dysfunction of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia revisited. Cereb Cortex. 2000 Nov;10(11):1078-92.
  • 15 Owen AM, McMillan KM, Laird AR, Bullmore E. N-back working memory paradigm: a meta-analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies. Hum Brain Mapp. 2005 May;25(1):46-59.
  • 16 Alvarez JA, Emory E. Executive function and the frontal lobes: a metaanalytic review. Neuropsychol Rev. 2006 Mar;16(1):17-42.
  • 17 Cornblatt BA, Risch NJ, Faris G, Friedman D, Erlenmeyer-Kimling L. The continuous performance test, identical pairs version (CPT-IP): I. new findings about sustained attention in normal families. Psychiatry Res. 1988 Nov;26(2):223-38.
  • 18 Krause AJ, Simon EB, Mander BA, Greer SM, Saletin JM, Goldstein- Piekarski AN, et al. The sleep-deprived human brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2017 Jul;18(7):404-18.
  • 19 Lim J, Dinges DF. A meta-analysis of the impact of short-term sleep deprivation on cognitive variables. Psychol Bull. 2010 May;136(3):375-89.
  • 20 Berry RB, Brooks R, Gamaldo CE, Harding SM, Lloyd RM, Marcus CL, et al. The AASM manual for the scoring of sleep and associated events: rules, terminology and technical specifications, version 2.1. Darien: American Academy of Sleep Medicine (AASM); 2014.
  • 21 Callicott JH, Mattay VS, Bertolino A, Finn K, Coppola R, Frank JA, et al. Physiological characteristics of capacity constraints in working memory as revealed by functional MRI. Cereb Cortex. 1999 Jan/Feb;9(1):20-6.
  • 22 Jacola LM, Willard VW, Ashford JM, Ogg RJ, Scoggins MA, Jones MM, et al. Clinical utility of the N-back task in functional neuroimaging studies of working memory. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2014 Oct;36(8):875-86.
  • 23 Tomida K, Takahashi N, Saito S, Maeno N, Iwamoto K, Yoshida K, et al. Relationship of psychopathological symptoms and cognitive function to subjective quality of life in patients with chronic schizophrenia. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2010 Jan;64(1):62-9.
  • 24 Banno M, Koide T, Aleksic B, Yamada K, Kikuchi T, Kohmura K, et al. A case control association study and cognitive function analysis of neuropilin and tolloid-like 1 gene and schizophrenia in the Japanese population. PLoS One. 2011;6(12):e28929.
  • 25 Sauvet F, Florence G, Van Beers P, Drogou C, Lagrume C, Chaumes C, et al. Total sleep deprivation alters endothelial function in rats: a nonsympathetic mechanism. Sleep. 2014 Mar;37(3):465-73.
  • 26 Irwin MR, Wang M, Campomayor CO, Collado-Hidalgo A, Cole S. Sleep deprivation and activation of morning levels of cellular and genomic markers of inflammation. Arch Intern Med. 2006 Sep;166(16):1756-62.
  • 27 Ramesh V, Nair D, Zhang SX, Hakim F, Kaushal N, Kayali F, et al. Disrupted sleep without sleep curtailment induces sleepiness and cognitive dysfunction via the tumor necrosis factor-α pathway. J Neuroinflammation. 2012;9:91.
  • 28 Baud MO, Magistretti PJ, Petit JM. Sustained sleep fragmentation affects brain temperature, food intake and glucose tolerance in mice. J Sleep Res. 2013 Feb;22(1):3-12.
  • 29 Petit JM, Gyger J, Burlet-Godinot S, Fiumelli H, Martin JL, Magistretti PJ. Genes involved in the astrocyte-neuron lactate shuttle (ANLS) are specifically regulated in cortical astrocytes following sleep deprivation in mice. Sleep. 2013 Oct;36(10):1445-58.
  • 30 Bishop NA, Lu T, Yankner BA. Neural mechanisms of ageing and cognitive decline. Nature. 2010 Mar;464(7288):529-35.
  • 31 Bliwise DL, Ansari FP, Straight LB, Parker KP. Age changes in timing and 24-hour distribution of self-reported sleep. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2005 Dec;13(12):1077-82.
  • 32 Mander BA, Winer JR, Walker MP. Sleep and human aging. Neuron. 2017 Apr;94(1):19-36.
  • 33 Chen X, Gelaye B, Williams MA. Sleep characteristics and health-related quality of life among a national sample of American young adults: assessment of possible health disparities. Qual Life Res. 2014 Mar;23(2):613-25.
  • 34 Blackwell T, Yaffe K, Ancoli-Israel S, Redline S, Ensrud KE, Stefanick ML, et al. Association of sleep characteristics and cognition in older communitydwelling men: the MrOS sleep study. Sleep. 2011 Oct;34(10):1347-56.
  • 35 Martin SE, Wraith PK, Deary IJ, Douglas NJ. The effect of nonvisible sleep fragmentation on daytime function. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 1997 May;155(5):1596-601.
  • 36 Kuriyama K, Mishima K, Suzuki H, Aritake S, Uchiyama M. Sleep accelerates the improvement in working memory performance. J Neurosci. 2008 Oct;28(40):10145-50.
  • 37 Fogel SM, Smith CT. The function of the sleep spindle: a physiological index of intelligence and a mechanism for sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011 Apr;35(5):1154-65.
  • 38 Schabus M, Hodlmoser K, Gruber G, Sauter C, Anderer P, Klosch G, et al. Sleep spindle-related activity in the human EEG and its relation to general cognitive and learning abilities. Eur J Neurosci. 2006 Apr;23(7):1738-46.
  • 39 Schabus M, Dang-Vu TT, Albouy G, Balteau E, Boly M, Carrier J, et al. Hemodynamic cerebral correlates of sleep spindles during human non-rapid eye movement sleep. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007 Aug;104(32):13164-9.
  • 40 Clemens Z, Molle M, Eross L, Jakus R, Rasonyi G, Halasz P, et al. Finetuned coupling between human parahippocampal ripples and sleep spindles. Eur J Neurosci. 2011 Feb;33(3):511-20.
  • 41 Sasaki T, Piatti VC, Hwaun E, Ahmadi S, Lisman JE, Leutgeb S, et al. Dentate network activity is necessary for spatial working memory by supporting CA3 sharp-wave ripple generation and prospective firing of CA3 neurons. Nat Neurosci. 2018 Feb;21(2):258-69.
  • 42 Lafortune M, Gagnon JF, Martin N, Latreille V, Dube J, Bouchard M, et al. Sleep spindles and rapid eye movement sleep as predictors of next morning cognitive performance in healthy middle-aged and older participants. J Sleep Res. 2014 Apr;23(2):159-67.
  • 43 Lau EYY, Wong ML, Lau KN, Hui FWY, Tseng CH. Rapid-eyemovement- sleep (REM) associated enhancement of working memory performance after a daytime nap. PLoS One. 2015;10(5):e0125752.
  • 44 Nofzinger EA. Functional neuroimaging of sleep. Semin Neurol. 2005 Mar;25(1):9-18.
  • 45 Baddeley A. The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory?. Trends Cogn Sci. 2000 Nov;4(11):417-23.
  • 46 Ferrarelli F, Kaskie R, Laxminarayan S, Ramakrishnan S, Reifman J, Germain A. An increase in sleep slow waves predicts better working memory performance in healthy individuals. Neuroimage. 2019 May;191:1-9.
  • 47 Pugin F, Metz AJ, Wolf M, Achermann P, Jenni OG, Huber R. Local increase of sleep slow wave activity after three weeks of working memory training in children and adolescents. Sleep. 2015 Apr;38(4):607-14.
  • 48 Baddeley A. Working memory. Science. 1992 Jan;255(5044):556-9.
  • 49 Evans KL, Hampson E. Sex differences on prefrontally-dependent cognitive tasks. Brain Cogn. 2015;93:42-53.
  • 50 Birchler-Pedross A, Schroder CM, Munch M, Knoblauch V, Blatter K, Schnitzler-Sack C, et al. Subjective well-being is modulated by circadian phase, sleep pressure, age, and gender. J Biol Rhythms. 2009 Jun;24(3):232-42.
  • 51 Van Der Helm E, Gujar N, Walker MP. Sleep deprivation impairs the accurate recognition of human emotions. Sleep. 2010 Mar;33(3):335-42.
  • 52 Ferri R, Drago V, Arico D, Bruni O, Remington RW, Stamatakis K, et al. The effects of experimental sleep fragmentation on cognitive processing. Sleep Med. 2010 Apr;11(4):378-85.