J Am Acad Audiol 2001; 12(02): 86-100
DOI: 10.1055/s-0042-1745584
Original Article

Lexical Effects on Dichotic Word Recognition in Young and Elderly Listeners

Anne Strouse Carter
Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Mountain Home, Tennessee, Departments of Surgery and Communicative Disorders, East Tennessee State University, Johnson City, Tennessee
,
Richard H. Wilson
Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Mountain Home, Tennessee, Departments of Surgery and Communicative Disorders, East Tennessee State University, Johnson City, Tennessee
› Institutsangaben

Abstract

Dichotic listening was evaluated using monosyllabic word pairs that differed in lexical difficulty as defined by the Neighborhood Activation Model of spoken word recognition. Four combinations of lexically EASY and lexically HARD words were evaluated (same pair: EASY-EASY, HARD-HARD; or mixed pair: EASY-HARD, HARD-EASY) in young adult listeners with normal hearing and older adult listeners with mild-to-moderate hearing loss. The same-pair data indicated that for all subjects, EASY words were identified correctly more often than HARD words, and recognition performance on words presented to the right ear was better than performance on words presented to the left ear. Overall performance was lower and the right-ear advantage was larger for the older group. The mixed-pair data for the young group revealed that EASY words were recognized more accurately than HARD words, regardless of the ear to which they were presented. For the older adults, the words presented to the right ear were recognized more accurately than were the words presented to the left ear, regardless of the type of word. The efficiency of the processing of stimuli from the left ear is discussed as an explanation of the results for the mixed-pair conditions.

Abbreviations: ANCOVA = analysis of covariance, ANOVA = analysis of variance, CV = consonant-vowel, NAM = Neighborhood Activation Model



Publikationsverlauf

Artikel online veröffentlicht:
28. Februar 2022

© 2001. American Academy of Audiology. This article is published by Thieme.

Thieme Medical Publishers, Inc.
333 Seventh Avenue, 18th Floor, New York, NY 10001, USA

 
  • REFERENCES

  • American National Standards Institute. (1996). American National Standard Specification for Audiometers. (ANSI S3–1996). New York: ANSI.
  • Balota DA, Duchek JM. (1991). Semantic priming effects, lexical repetition effects, and contextual disambiguation effects in healthy aged individuals and individuals with senile dementia of the Alzheimer type. Brain Lang 40:181–201.
  • Bellis TJ. (1996). Assessment and Management of Central Auditory Processing Disorders in the Educational Setting. San Diego: Singular.
  • Broadbent DE. (1954). The role of auditory localization in attention and memory span. J Exp Psychol 47:191–196.
  • Broadbent DE. (1967). Word-frequency effect and response bias. Psychol Rev 14:1–15.
  • Bryden MP. (1963). Ear preference in auditory perception. J Exp Psychol 65:103–105.
  • Bryden MP. (1988). An overview of the dichotic listening procedure and its relation to cerebral organization. In: Hugdahl K, ed. Handbook of Dichotic Listening: Theory, Methods, and Research. New York: John Wiley and Sons, 1–43.
  • Cluff MS, Luce PA. (1990). Similarity neighborhoods of spoken two-syllable words: retroactive effects on multiple activation. J Exp Psychol 3:551–563.
  • Department of Veterans Affairs. (1998). Speech Recognition and Identification Materials, Disc 2.0. Mountain Home, TN: VA Medical Center.
  • Dirks DD. (1964). Perception of dichotic and monaural verbal material and cerebral dominance for speech. Acta Otolaryngol (Stockh) 58:73–80.
  • Efron R. (1985). The central auditory system and issues related to hemispheric specialization. In: Pinheiro ML, Musiek FE, eds. Assessment of Central Auditory Dysfunction: Foundations and Clinical Correlates. Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins, 143–154.
  • Goldstein SG, Braun LS. (1974). Reversal of expected transfer as a function of increased age. Percept Mot Skills 38:1139–1145.
  • Goldstein G, Shelly C. (1981). Does the right hemisphere age more rapidly than the left? J Clin Neuropsychol 3:67–78.
  • Gordon B. (1983). Lexical access and lexical decision: mechanisms of frequency sensitivity. J Verbal Learn Verbal Behav 18:24–44.
  • Hasher L, Stoltzfus ER, Zacks RT, Rypma B. (1991). Age and inhibition. J Exp Psychol 17:163–169.
  • Hugdahl K. (1988). Handbook of Dichotic Listening: Theory, Methods and Research. New York: Wiley.
  • Jerger J, Alford B, Lew H, Rivera V, Chmiel R. (1995). Dichotic listening, event-related potentials, and inter-hemispheric transfer in the elderly. Ear Hear 16:482–98.
  • Jerger J, Stach B, Johnson K, Loiselle L, Jerger S. (1990). Patterns of abnormality in dichotic listening. In: Jensen J, ed. Presbyacusis and Other Age Related Aspects. Copenhagen: Stougaard Jensen, 143–150.
  • Johnson RC, Cole RE, Bowers JK, Foiles SV, Patrick JW, Woliver RE. (1979). Hemisphere efficiency in middle and later adulthood. Cortex 15:109–119.
  • Kimura D. (1961). Cerebral dominance and the perception of verbal stimuli. Can J Psychol 15:156–165.
  • Kimura D. (1967). Functional asymmetry of the brain in dichotic listening. Cortex 3:163–178.
  • Kirk KI, Pisoni DB, Miyamoto RC. (1997). Effects of stimulus variability on speech perception in listeners with hearing impairment. J Speech Lang Hear Res 40:1395–1405.
  • Kirk KI, Pisoni DB, Sommers MS, Young M, Evanson C. (1995). New directions for assessing speech perception in persons with sensory aids. Ann Otol Rhinol Laryngol Suppl 166:300–303.
  • Kucera F, Francis W. (1967). Computational Analysis of Present Day American English. Providence, RI: Brown University Press.
  • Lake DA, Bryden MP. (1976). Handedness and sex differences in hemispheric asymmetry. Brain Lang 3:266–282.
  • Luce PA. (1986). Neighborhoods of Words in the Mental Iexicon. (Technical Report No. 6). Bloomington, IN: Speech Research Laboratory, Indiana University.
  • Luce PA, Pisoni DB. (1998). Recognizing spoken words: the neighborhood activation model. Ear Hear 19:1–36.
  • Luce PA, Pisoni DB, Goldinger SD. (1990). Similarity neighborhoods of spoken words. In: Altmann GT, ed. Cognitive Models of Speech Processing: Psycholinguistic and Computational Perspectives. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 122–147.
  • McKeever WF, Nolan DR, Diehi JA, Seitz KS. (1984). Handedness and language laterality: discrimination of handedness groups on the dichotic consonant-vowel task. Cortex 20:509–523.
  • Meyer TA, Pisoni DB. (1999). Some computational analyses of the PBK test: effects of frequency and lexical density on spoken word recognition. Ear Hear 20:363–371.
  • Musiek FE. (1983). Assessment of central auditory dysfunction: the dichotic digit test revisited. Ear Hear 4:79–83.
  • Musiek FE, Gollegly KM, Baran JA. (1984). Myelination of the corpus callosum and auditory processing in children: theoretical and clinical correlates. Semin Hear 5:231–241.
  • Musiek FE, Gollegly KM, Kibbe KS, Verkest-Lenz SB. (1991). Proposed screening test for central auditory disorders: follow-up on the dichotic digits test. Am J Otol 12:109–113.
  • Nusbaum HC, Pisoni DB, Davis CK. (1984). Sizing Up the Hoosier Mental Lexicon: Measuring the Familiarity of 20,000 Words—Research on Speech Perception. Progress report No. 10. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University, Department of Psychology.
  • Oldfield RC. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychology 9:97–113.
  • Soloman RL, Postman L. (1952). Frequency of usage as a determinant of recognition thresholds for words. J Exp Psychol 43:195–201.
  • Sommers MS. (1996). The structural organization of the mental lexicon and its contribution to age-related declines in spoken-word recognition. Psychol Aging 11:333–341.
  • Sommers MS. (1999). Spoken word recognition in individuals with dementia of the Alzheimer's type: changes in talker normalization and lexical discrimination. Psychol Aging 13:631–646.
  • Sommers MS, Danielson SM. (1999). Inhibitory processes and spoken word recognition in young and older adults:the interaction of lexical competition and semantic content. Psychol Aging 14:458–472.
  • Sommers MS, Kirk KI, Pisoni DB. (1997). Some considerations in evaluating spoken word recognition by normal-hearing, noised masked normal-hearing and cochlear implant listeners. I: The effects of response format. Ear Hear 18:89–99.
  • Sparks R, Goodglass H, Nickel B. (1970). Ipsilateral versus contralateral extinction in dichotic listening resulting from hemisphere lesions. Cortex 6:249–260.
  • Speaks CE. (1975). Dichotic listening: a clinical or research tool? In: Sullivan MD, ed. Proceedings of a Symposium on Central Auditory Processing Disorders. Omaha, NE: University of Nebraska Medical Center, 1–25.
  • Speaks C, Niccum N, Carney E. (1982). Statistical properties of responses to dichotic listening with CV nonsense syllables. J Acoust Soc Am 72:1185–1194.
  • Speaks C, Niccum N, Van Tasell D. (1985). Effects of stimulus material on the dichotic listening performance of patients with sensorineural hearing loss. J Speech Hear Res 28:16–25.
  • Strouse AL, Hall JWH. (1995). Test-retest reliability of a dichotic digits test for assessing central auditory function in Alzheimer's disease. Audiology 34:85–90.
  • Strouse A, Wilson RH. (1999a). Stimulus length uncertainty with dichotic digit recognition. J Am Acad Audiol 10:219–229.
  • Strouse A, Wilson RH. (1999b). Recognition of 1–, 2–, and 3–pair dichotic digits under free and directed recall. J Am Acad Audiol 10:557–571.
  • Wilson RH, Jaffe MS. (1996). Interactions of age, ear, and stimulus complexity on dichotic digit recognition. J Am Acad Audiol 7:358–364.
  • Wilson RH, Leigh ED. (1996). Identification performance by right- and left-handed listeners on dichotic CV material. J Am Acad Audiol 7:1–6.